source : jeudepaume.org |
Rudolf Flesch, Why Johnny Still Can't Read (1981)
1
Why Johnny Still Can’t Read
Are you worrying about
your child's education? You should be. There's an 85 percent chance that your
Johnny or Mary will never learn to read properly.
There are two schools of
thought about how to teach a child to read. One is called "intensive
phonies" or "systematic phonies" or, more recently,
"decoding" or "code emphasis." In this book, to avoid
confusion, I'll call it "phonics-first." The other is called the
"look-and-say" or "whole-word" or "sight-reading"
method or—so help me—"psycholinguistics." I'll use
"look-and-say."
When I wrote my book Why Johnny Can't Read[1]
twenty-five years ago, look-and-say ruled supreme. Almost all American
schools used it. Phonics-first was a poor orphan, used only in a handful of
schools.
I said in my book that
phonics-first worked splendidly and should be used in all schools, while
look-and-say was wretchedly poor and should be abandoned at once.
Unfortunately my advice
fell on deaf ears. With heart-breaking slowness, phonics-first crept into some
15 percent of our schools, but an estimated 85 percent of them still stick to
old, discredited look-and-say.
The results of this mass
miseducation have been disastrous. America is rapidly sinking into a morass of
ignorance. The official statistics are appalling.
In 1975 the U.S. Office of
Education sponsored the so-called APL (Adult Performance Level) study,
conducted by Dr. [2] Norvel Northcutt of the
University of Texas in Austin. It was designed to find out how many Americans
had the skills to cope with modern life. It showed that 21.7 percent of adults
between eighteen and sixty-five—or 23 million people—couldn't read a want ad, a
job application form, a label on a medicine bottle, or a safety sign at a
workplace.
Of those 23
million, 16 percent had never gone beyond third grade. This left 19 million
who'd had four or more years of schooling but never learned how to read. Why?
Because almost all of them were taught by look-and-say, and the method doesn't
work.
And what happened
at the other end of the educational scale? How did our brightest young people
do at college age after they'd been taught reading by look-and-say in first
grade? For them, look-and-say worked like a time bomb. In 1963 the nationwide
college entrance test (Scholastic Aptitude Test or SAT) scores began to drop.
They've been dropping steeply ever since, with no end in sight. Verbal SAT
averages, which stood at 478 in 1963, were down to 424 in 1980—a staggering
drop of 9 percent of the whole 200-800-point range in seventeen years.
In 1977 a
blue-ribbon twenty-one-member advisory panel, headed by former Secretary of
Labor Willard Wirtz, issued a report on the decline in the SAT scores.
"The panel members," it said, "share strongly the national
concern about the increasing signs of functional illiteracy . . . [but they
could find] no one cause of the SAT
score decline."
However, in an
appendix to the report, Professor Jeanne Chall of Harvard University offered a
clue to that one basic cause. The students' low SAT scores, she wrote, had a
"clearcut" statistical relationship to the reading instruction
program used ten years earlier in first grade. The program had been based on a
series of "look-and-say" readers.
There's little
doubt that we'll soon have doctors who can't easily read medical journals,
lawyers who have difficulty researching a case, scientists who stumble through
their professional literature. In the 1990s we'll have to import top
professionals from abroad. We'll join the ranks of such undereducated Third
World countries as the Ivory Coast, Saudi Arabia, and Zambia. And there'll be
few, if any, Nobel Prize [3] winners who learned to read in an
American school.
I've earned the right to
say that these grim prospects are the direct results of look-and-say teaching
in our schools. Twenty-five years ago I studied American methods of teaching
reading and warned against educational catastrophe. Now it has happened.
Surely you don't want your
Johnny or Mary to grow up as a functional illiterate or educational cripple. In
this book I'll show you what you can do to help your child get a good
education.
I’ll start with the
difference between phonics-first and look-and-say.
Learning to read is like
learning to drive a car. You take lessons and learn the mechanics and the rules
of the road. After a few weeks you've learned how to drive, how to stop, how to
shift gears, how to park, and how to signal. You've also learned to stop at a
red light and understand road signs. When you're ready, you take a road test,
and if you pass, you can drive.
Phonics-first works the same
way. The child learns the mechanics of reading, and when he's through, he can
read.
Look-and-say works
differently. The child is taught to read before he's learned the mechanics—the
sounds of the letters. It's like learning to drive by starting your car and
driving ahead. You'd learn to recognize and remember certain landmarks. First,
on your street, you pass by the big yellow garage, the house with a plastic
stork on the lawn, and the dentist's house. You turn right and pass the
plumber, the florist, and the Italian restaurant. You turn again and drive by
the stationery store, the carpenter, the podiatrist, the funeral home, and the
bank. Then you come to the big intersection with the Exxon and the Texaco gas
stations. You turn again and pass the diner, the gift shop, the drugstore, the
optometrist, the little knit shop, and the pediatrician. One more turn and
you're back home.
Continuing that
"look-and-say" method of learning how to drive, you would repeat that
lesson for three or four months until you'd be fully familiar with all the
landmarks—the yellow garage, the plastic stork, the dentist, the plumber, the
florist, the Italian restaurant, the stationery store, the carpenter, the [4] podiatrist,
the funeral home, the bank, the Exxon station, the Texaco station, the diner,
the gift shop, the drugstore, the optometrist, the knit shop, and the
pediatrician. You'd have learned to drive around the block.
Then you'd be allowed to
go farther. Three months and you would have learned how to drive to the
Catholic church and the supermarket. By the time you've fully learned those
other landmarks, it would be the end of the school year and you would have a
"landmark vocabulary" of 350 items. Next year you'd learn to drive to
the railroad station and the Protestant church on the hill.
And the mechanics of
driving? You'd pick those up as you go along. After three months you'd learn
how to step on the brake, after another two months you'd learn how to signal.
Next year you'd learn about traffic lights.
Now let's see how these
two methods work with reading. With phonics-first the child is first taught the
letters of the alphabet and what sounds they stand for. Since English has only
twenty-six letters to express about forty-four sounds, this is done in a strict
sequence so that the child sees only words whose letter sounds he has already
learned. For instance, a sentence in the first Lippincott reader says,
"Ann and Dan pin up the map." Before they get to that sentence, the
children have learned the sounds of n,
d, p, m, short a, short i, and short u. They've
also learned the word the, one of a
handful of special words taught out of sequence to make it possible to tell a
story.
At the end of a semester
or a year or two years, depending on which phonic system you use, children can
read an estimated 24,000 words in their speaking or listening vocabulary. They
can then go on to grammar, composition, literature, social studies, and
science—in other words, they can start on their education.
And how does look-and-say
work? It works on the principle that children learn to read by reading. It
starts with little "stories" containing the most-often-used words in
English and gradually builds up a "sight vocabulary." The children
learn to read by seeing those words over and over again. By the end of first
grade they can recognize 349 words, by the end of second grade 1,094, by the
end of third grade 1,216, and by [5] the end of fourth
grade 1,554. (I got those numbers from the Scott, Foresman series, but all look-and-say
series teach about the same number of words.)
The Scott,
Foresman cumulative fourth-grade list contains the words anteater, chariot, freckle, Hawaiian, laryngitis, peccary, Siberian, skunk, and toothpick. But it does not contain the words boil, cell, cheap, church, coal, cost, crime, due, fact,
pain, pray, pride, puff, root, steam, stock, sum, tax, twelve, and vote. These are words a look-and-say-trained child may not be able to read by the end of fourth
grade. Of course, if he'd been taught phonics-first, he'd be able to read his
full speaking or listening vocabulary, which has been estimated at 40,000
words.
Look-and-say
readers still start the way they used to. I looked at a pre-primer of the 1979
Ginn 720 series and found myself right back in the early fifties. The same
repetitive little "stories," the same children, the same dog. The
dog's name is Lad, and the three white children, Bill, Jill, and Ben, have been
joined by two black children, Ted and Nan, and by Rosa, who is Hispanic. But
the stuff inside is much the same as it always was:
I
am Rosa.
I
am Lad.
Rosa
runs.
Bill
runs.
Lad
and Rosa go.
Rosa
and Jill go.
Go,
Rosa, go.
Go,
Bill, go.
There's one
difference between the look-and-say readers of twenty-five years ago and those
of today. Today they all offer some phonics. Not that they've gone over to the
phonics-first camp, but since millions of parents now clamor for phonics, they
give them a minimum of phonics—served up in a look-and-say sauce of
"context clues" and guesswork.
Unfortunately,
this bit of window dressing does the children no good at all. They still don't
learn phonics before they learn the
words spelled by the phonic rules. Instead, they get a little phonics that's
too late, incomplete, thrown in more or less at [6] random. Along with being
taught to read those measly 1,500 words, they're taught some phonics as if it
were a separate subject like math.
For instance, the
first line in the 1978 Scott, Foresman look-and-say pre-primer says, "Look
and Listen." When are the children taught the sound of double o as in look? Two years later, when they're in third grade. Lesson 10 in
their third-grade reader starts, "When you see a word with the letters oo . . ."
And when do they
learn about the silent t in listen? I plowed through the whole
sixteen-volume Scott, Foresman series, but in vain. Silent t is never explained. Nor are ph
and qu, even though the
first-grade reader contains the words elephant
and queen.
All look-and-say
readers do the same thing. In the Ginn 720 series, page 36 of the first-grade
reader is wholly devoted to teaching the word this. The phonic
"explanation" of the word is given one-and-a-half years later, in second grade. How is it done? By
asking the children to circle a word to complete the sentence "I like bath
/ car best." (Phonics window dressing this_ taught by a look-and-say
"context clue.")
In another Ginn
720 first-grade book they teach the word guess
by repeating it eight times in a 124-word story. And when does the Ginn
series get around to teaching the sound of gu
as in guess, guest, or guide? Three years later, in fourth grade.
Again, in a 1979
Houghton Mifflin first-grade reader, children learn the word bus early in the year. When are they
taught the sound of short u? One-and-a-half years later, in second
grade. Then the teacher is told to put the words up, but, stuck, and lunch on the blackboard and tell the children, "Listen for the
vowel you hear in each word."
In the same
second-grade reader, the children are taught to memorize the word enough. And when are they taught the f sound of gh in enough, rough, tough, and laugh? The
answer is, Never. Houghton Mifflin simply doesn't bother with such trifles.
So, in spite of all
the phonic window dressing, look-and-say still teaches reading by pictures, by
telling what the word means, or simply by letting the children guess. Since all
those [7] methods have long been thoroughly discredited, they're disguised.
Instead of
telling the children outright what a word means, teachers are told in a 1979
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich teacher's manual: "Write the word buy on the
chalkboard. Point to the word and ask the children to pronounce it. If
necessary, say Buy begins like boy and
rhymes with why.' "
Instead of
referring the children to the picture of a king they have in front of them,
teachers are told, in a Scott, Foresman manual: "Point to the word king and say, `How do you know that this
word is king and not kitchen?' "The children are supposed to answer "Because of the
context," but of course they can tell by the picture.
A classic example
of the look-and-say method is this instruction to teachers in a 1979 Houghton
Mifflin third-grade manual:
Print disturb on the board and say, "You
can find out what this word is. I am going to say a sentence but leave out this
word at the end. When I stop, use what you know about the sounds the letters
stand for to help you think of a word that would make sense with the other
words. Here's the sentence: When I went past my brother's bed-room, I noticed
that his door was shut and there was a sign up saying, 'do not ________ .' What is the word? . . . How do you know it
isn't disposal? . . . ('Disposal doesn't make sense with what
you said.') How do you know it isn't interfere?
. . . ('interfere doesn't begin with
the sound that d stands for.')"
When I went to
Teachers College, Columbia University, I was taught that the worst thing a
teacher can do is suggest mistakes. But the look-and-say educators love this
device. It's in all their teachers' manuals. For instance:
Print: handkerchief
Say: "He reached into his pocket and pulled out a ________.
Checking words: handicap (no
sense), wallet (wrong sounds).
[8] Or
this:
Print: salad
Say: "For lunch I ate a vegetable____
Checking words: salary (no sense)
Checking words: salary (no sense)
casserole
(wrong sounds)
When I read this, my
twenty-five-year-old anger came flooding back. Just think of it! At the end of third grade students are not expected to
be able to read the word salad. Salad! A word that a phonics-first
school teaches in the first weeks of first grade!
But the look-and-say
educators don't care. They know full well that after three years their students
are apt to read salad as salary or casserole. Why? Because they've kept it a closely guarded secret
that the word salad can be read by sounding
out the letters s, a, 1,
a, d.
Did I say three years? I'm
sorry I misled you. Many look-and-say-taught children never catch on to the letter-sound relationships of the English
language. I can prove this—right out of the horse's mouth.
The Houghton Mifflin
series of readers runs from kindergarten through eighth grade. In 1978 the
publishers realized that junior high school English teachers may not be aware
of the abominable situation in the lower grades. They may naïvely assume that
all their students can read.
To deal with this problem,
Houghton Mifflin inserted a full-page, boldface statement opposite the title
page in both their seventh- and eighth-grade teachers' manuals. The statement
is called An Important Statement
Regarding the Decoding of Words Strange Only in Printed Form. I quote:
Some students have not yet learned how to decode easily and
quickly the printed form of the language into the oral form with which they are
thoroughly familiar. . . . They are probably not sufficiently aware that any
specific reading passage consists of letter symbols in a sort of secret code....
They almost certainly lack mastery of a reliable strategy for decoding printed
language. . . . For those students . . . provide the necessary instruction
recommended on pages 455-458.
[9] Translated
into plain English, this means:
Perhaps, as a junior high school English teacher, you don't know
that some students in your class have been taught by look-and-say and don't
know how to read. Many of them have never caught on to the fact that letters
stand for sounds. Please do the exercises in the back of this manual with them
to help them learn to read.
Sure enough, in the back
of the seventh- and eighth-grade manuals are some quite useless instructions on
how to teach, seven years too late,
such words as demonstrated, rear, plumber, costume, gleam, stumbled, mending, pool, and brake. The word brake,
for instance, is to be taught this way:
Context: "The car began to roll down the hill. Thinking
quickly, Sally jumped into the car and put her foot on the brake."
Questions: "How do you know the word is not pedal?" "How do you know the
word is not broom?"
So now it's official. The
leading look-and-say publisher has let the cat out of the bag. Millions of
junior high school students, taught by look-and-say, can't distinguish the
words brake, pedal, and broom.
The moral of all this is
clear. Go to your child's school tomorrow morning and find out what system it
uses to teach reading. Check the textbooks against these two lists:
THE PHONIC FIVE
1.
Addison-Wesley Publishing Co.,
2725 Sand Hill Road, Menlo Park, Calif. 94825
2.
Distar, Science Research Associates,
259 East Erie Street, Chicago, Ill. 60611
3.
Economy Company, 1901 North
Walnut Street, Oklahoma City, Okla. 73125
4.
J. B. Lippincott Company, East
Washington Square, Philadelphia, Pa. 19105
5.
Open Court Publishing Co., P.O.
Box 599, La Salle, 61301
[10] THE DISMAL DOZEN
1.
Allyn & Bacon, Inc.
2.
American Book Company
3.
Ginn & Company
4.
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich
5.
Harper & Row
6.
Holt, Rinehart & Winston
7.
Houghton Mifflin Company
8.
Laidlaw Brothers
9.
Macmillan, Inc. (regular
series)
10.
Macmillan, Inc. (Bank
Street Readers)
11.
Riversi de Publishing
Co. ( Rand McNally & Co. ) Scott, Foresman & Company
If the school
uses one of the Phonic Five series, Johnny or Mary will be all right. They'll soon
read better than most American children.
But if the school
uses one of the Dismal Dozen, you have a problem. Johnny or Mary may never
learn to read without fumbling and stumbling.
Here's what you
can do about it.
To begin with,
give Johnny first aid. Since he won't be taught phonics-first in school, teach
him phonics at home. Don't tell me you can't do it. It's only the look-and-say
educators who have blown this thing out of all proportion and have made the
teaching of reading seem like going to the moon. Actually, it's quite simple.
All you need is a series of step-by-step exercises plus time and patience.
Millions of nineteenth century Americans taught their children to read the same
way with the aid of the famous little Webster's Blue-Backed Speller. Tens of
thousands of mothers and fathers have done it successfully with the help of my
book Why Johnny Can't Read—And What You
Can Do About It (Harper & Row, 1955).
Of course you can do it. So can
your parents, your older children, your twelve-year-old babysitter (if she's
been taught phonics-first). I've taught each of my six children that way, each
before he or she entered school, and those were among the happiest and most
satisfying experiences of my life. It's [11]
an unforgettable moment when a child first discovers the key to
the "secret code."
However, even if you teach
your Johnny successfully at home, he'll still be exposed to the poor education
he's getting at his look-and-say school. He'll go to school with lots of children
who can't read. He'll learn from textbooks that were carefully "dumbed
down" one, two, or more grades. He'll attend feather-weight courses
tailored to educational cripples. If possible, get him out of there and into a
phonics-first school. Their number is growing, and if you're lucky, you'll find
one not far from your home.
Don't think this
suggestion is frivolous. I'm very serious about it. With our schools the way
they are, this is possibly the most important thing you can do for your child's
future career and happiness. If I had a child in a look-and-say school, I'd
gladly have him bused thirty miles to be taught phonics-first.
But suppose you can't do
that. What then? In that case I suggest you try to make your school switch from
the Dismal Dozen to one of the Phonic Five.
You say you have never
done such a thing? Well, there's always the first time to take a hand in
community affairs. Become an active citizen. Go to the school board. Start
hollering. Ring doorbells. Organize meetings. Where there's a will, there's a
way. Thousands of parents throughout the nation have done it and so can you.
Witness the famous case of the parents of Rochester, New York—black and
white—who forced the City to adopt phonics-first readers. Dozens of other
stories tell of parents who overcame a stubborn superintendent or won a
majority on the school board—all to ensure that their children were taught
phonics-first.
To give you an idea of
what to expect if you succeed, I made arrangements to visit a phonics-first
school. As soon as I contacted the five phonics publishers, I got lists of
hundreds of schools that would be glad to have me visit their classrooms. (I
wonder if any look-and-say schools proudly open their class-rooms to observers
of what's going on.)
To save time, I decided to
visit a school nearby and picked at random a phonics-first school in New York
City. Yes, I know what you're going to say. New York, as everyone knows, is an
educational desert where reading test scores have been [12]
dropping year after year. But some ten years ago the city's school
principals were at long last given a free hand and allowed to escape from the
deadly grip of look-and-say. A few of them chose phonics-first and almost
overnight made the desert bloom with educational "miracles."
One such school is P.S.
251, deep in the heart of Brooklyn. It serves the Paerdegat district, whose
children are Irish, Italian, Jewish, black, Puerto Rican, Chinese, Korean,
Haitian, Cuban—a great American ethnic mix.
I visited P.S. 251 in May
1979. The principal, Mrs. Cynthia Kamen, gave me a complete guided tour through
a dozen or more classrooms. I saw a succession of "miracles"—only of
course in a phonics-first school miracles happen every day. P.S. 251 uses the
Open Court Publishing Company system and its pupils start to prepare for
reading in kindergarten.
Each of the classrooms was
filled with wide-awake, self-confident children. Each question produced a
forest of eagerly raised hands.
I went into a kindergarten
classroom. The children had learned the whole alphabet and treated me to
rousing versions of the "H-song" and "W-song."
In first and second grade,
the children proved to me that they could read anything. I had brought a list
of six test words—flamingo, curlicue, delicacy, inert, stoic, and squabble. They read those words without trouble and pronounced them
correctly.
Next I showed them a
headline from that morning's New York
Times. It said:
Senate Confirmation Role Sought
On Posts of Brzezinski and Aide
Out of five first- and
second-graders I showed that headline to, four read and pronounced all the
words correctly, including the name of President Carter's national security
adviser. I remember particularly little Theresa in second grade, who rattled
off the name as if it were her own.
My daughter Abby went with
me on my trip to P.S. 251 and can testify under oath that these
"miracles" actually happened.
Next, in a third-grade
classroom, we listened to a bright boy [13]
named Steven reading aloud a story he'd written. Its first paragraph
read:
Gus is a flying hippo. He is big, fat, and klutzy. He is kelly
green with lopsided spots. Just look for his crimson face peering of you. Last
seen he was wearing a bright purple sweater. He talks like a hypochondriac.
Other third-graders read
to us poems from the third-grade Open Court reader. They read
"Daffodils" by William Wordsworth and "The Lost Shoe" by
Walter de la Mare. They had no problems with Wordsworth's words continuous, margin, and sprightly, or
with Walter de la Mare's references to Hindustan and Pernambuco.
The children in the fourth
grade had read twenty books in the Random House Landmark series about
historical figures and were eager to tell us about Benjamin Franklin and Clara
Barton.
Then we looked in on a
sixth grade, where the students were rehearsing a play. Among the books they'd
read were plays by Thornton Wilder and William Saroyan, Jane Eyre, Great Expectations,
and The Diary of Anne Frank.
We next went into a few
classrooms where specially trained teachers tutored emotionally disturbed and
learning-disabled children, transferred from other schools. They are taught
with immense patience and loving attention to each child. Their progress is
slow, but P.S. 251 simply doesn't tolerate nonreaders.
The school also has
"magnet" classes for gifted children. We spent a happy quarter of an
hour in a kindergarten filled with bright kids who read from a long list of
outlandish words their teacher had taught them—words like digressing, ecology, decorous, and dactylology. I didn't know what that last word meant and the
children explained to me that it meant hand-sign language. They also knew the
definitions of all the other words. A vow
meant "a promise," a kleptomaniac
was "someone who steals and can't help it," a phenomenon was "like a miracle."
When we came to the word mandatory, the
teacher asked them whether kindergarten was mandatory. Happily they chorused,
"No!!!"
It was 12:30 and we
prepared to leave. But then Mrs. Kamen [14]
casually mentioned a fourth-grade class whose teacher, Mrs. Mildred Cohen,
taught a special unit about Greek mythology. That I had to see. So we went into the fourth-grade classroom. The
children were asked to tell us the mythological stories they'd learned.
Nine-year-old Mark told us with vim and vigor about Persephone, the daughter of
Demeter, who went to Hades and brought back seven pomegranates. I interrupted
Mark and asked him to spell pomegranate.
He did so, hardly breaking his stride, and went on with his story. Next,
charming little Shari told us about Prometheus stealing fire. She casually
mentioned his obscure brother Epimetheus, pronouncing the name without the
slightest hesitation.
Mrs. Cohen
proudly gave us a collection of poems the children had written about the Greek
gods. I particularly liked this one by nine-year-old Ruth:
Nectar
Zeus
drinks nectar
The
Gods eat ambrosia
The
Gods live on Mount Olympus
Way
up
[1] Bibliographical
information on references will be found by chapter at the end of the text,
starting at page 171.
References
Chapter 1: Why Johnny Still Can't Read
Page
1 Flesch, Rudolf. Why
Johnny Can't Read—And What You Can Do About It. New York, Harper & Row,
1955.
1 Final Report on the Adult
Performance Level Study. University of Texas at Austin, 1975.
2 Maeroff, Gene L. "Scores on Scholastic Aptitude Tests Continue
to Drop." New York Times, October
5, 1980, p. 29.
2 On Further Examination. College
Entrance Examination Board. New York, 1977.
2 Chall, Jeanne S.; Conrad, S. S.; and Harris, S. H. An Analysis of Textbooks in Relation to
Declining SAT Scores. College Entrance Examination Board. New York, 1977,
p. 62.
Aucun commentaire:
Enregistrer un commentaire